by Dik Bakker
This map gives a survey of the distribution of languages with person markers on their adpositions. An example of a person-marked preposition is found in (1) from Maybrat (West Papuan; Papua, Indonesia).
T-ai
m-kah
ara.
1sg-hit
3sg.n-with
stick
‘I hit with a stick.’
The 378 languages in the map have been characterized in terms of the four values in the table below.
Go to map | ||
---|---|---|
Value | Representation | |
No adpositions | 63 | |
Adpositions without person marking | 209 | |
Person marking for pronouns only | 83 | |
Person marking for pronouns and nouns | 23 | |
Total: | 378 |
In order to determine whether a language has person marking on its adpositions, we first need to establish whether it possesses the category adposition at all. In general, the major function of an adposition is to relate its object, i.e. the noun phrase with which it forms a constituent, to another nominal or a verbal constituent on the basis of a more or less specific semantic relationship, such as location, time, property, instrument or possession. Languages may use several strategies to fulfill this function, either lexical or morphological. In example (2) below, from Barbareño Chumash (California), we have a verbal construction fulfilling such a function. What is expressed by an adverb and an adposition in the English translation, across from, is rendered by a fully inflected verb in this language.
(2) Barbareño Chumash (Wash 2001: 75)
Kʰ-ili-ʔetemé.su ̎s
hi
lwí.sa ̎
hiklé-ḱen
hi-ho-l̇amė́.sa.
1-hab-be.across.from
dep
Luisa
dep-1.sit
dep-dist-table
‘I used to sit across from Luisa at the table.’
Example (3), from Tauya (Madang; Papua New Guinea), shows a nominal strategy to express the locality relationship rendered by the preposition beside in the English version.
(3) Tauya (MacDonald 1990: 283)
ya
nai-sa
1sg
rib-loc
‘beside me’
Although diachronically forms such as the verb Kʰiliʔetemé.su ̎s in (2) and the noun naisa in (3) may eventually give rise to adpositions, they will not be considered as such here. In general a specific element in a language will be assumed to be an adposition only if it is morphologically independent and displays morphosyntactic behaviour distinct from more clearcut verbal, nominal or adverbial elements in that language.
In addition to languages such as Chumash and Tauya which encode adpositional meanings via verbs or nouns, there are languages in which the relevant relations are coded by morphological means, typically by a case suffix. The example from Arabana (Pama-Nyungan; South Australia) in (4) illustrates this strategy. The ablative case is used to express ‘away from’.
(4) Arabana (Hercus 1994: 71)
Maka-ru
kilta-rnda.
Fire-abl
pull-pres
‘He pulls it out of the fire.’
Another type of morphologically dependent expression of nominal relations is via adverbial nominal affixes, as in example (5) from Jaqaru (Aymaran; Peru).
(5) Jaqaru (Hardman 2000: 21)
ut-nuri-t”a
house-within-from
‘from within the house’
Arguably a borderline case between morphological and syntactic expression are clitics which attach to the noun phrase rather than to nouns, as exemplified in (6) from Ngankikurungkurr (Daly; Northern Territory, Australia).
(6) Ngankikurungkurr (Hoddinott and Kofod 1988: 72)
Kalla
ngayi
yedi
tye
jeningkisyi
yaga=nide.
Mother
1sg
went
pst
canoe
dem=loc
‘My mother came in that canoe.’
In many instances, case affixes as in (4) and clitics as in (6) can be analyzed diachronically as the result of the affixation or cliticization of adpositions. However, taking a rather conservative, strictly syntactic position on this matter, I will not consider such bound forms as true adpositions. A further argument for not doing so may be that since such forms are (syntactic) dependents rather than heads, they are not potential targets for person marking but, to the contrary, may attach themselves to pronominal forms. As a result of this, all languages which resort exclusively to strategies as exemplified under (2)-(6) are coded as having no adpositions (value one) on the map. They are by definition irrelevant for the type of person marking under consideration here.
Having established the criteria for what constitutes an adposition, we must now determine what qualifies as person marking on adpositions. In order for a potential marker to be considered a person marker it should fulfill two requirements. First, there should be enough differentiation between the relevant forms that a distinction exists either between all three persons or between any combination of first, second or third person. One of the forms may be zero. And secondly, the forms should be affixes rather than clitics.
Languages with only bare adpositions, and those for which markers on their adpositions do not meet the criteria mentioned above irrespective of the nature of the adpositional object, are assigned value two (adpositions without person marking). English is a case in point, and so is Polish, as shown in example (7a-b).
(7) Polish (Anna Siewierska p.c.)
a.
Idę
do
Kasi.
go.fut.1sg
to
Kasia.gen
‘I go to Kasia.’
b.
Idę
do
niej.
go.fut.1sg
to
3sg.f.gen
‘I go to her.’
When languages do show person marking on adpositions for pronominal objects but not for nominal objects, they are assigned value three (person marking for pronouns only). Paamese (Oceanic; Vanuatu) has this kind of adpositional marking, as shown in (8a-b) below.
(8) Paamese (Crowley 1982: 182)
a.
Ham
sān
lēta
min-nau
ranaut
Vīla.
Ham
3sg.send
letter
to-1sg
from.place
Vila
‘Mail Ham sent me a letter from Vila.’
b.
Kai
selūs
min
tāta
ven
mane
onak.
3sg
3sg.speak
to
father
about
money
poss.1sg
‘He spoke to father about my money.’
Typically, in these languages the adposition has no separate independent pronominal object apart from the person marker itself, although there may be one under certain conditions, such as contrastive stress. Hence, more often than not, the person marker on the adposition is the only element representing the pronominal object. In this group are also included languages for which only a subset of the adpositions have person marking. In Burushaski (isolate; Pakistan or India) the postposition pači ‘with’ in (9a) occurs with a prefixed person marker; the postposition gΛnε ‘for’ in (9b) does not.
(9) Burushaski (Lorimer 1935: 96, 97)
a.
Ja
aˑ-pʌči
huruˑt.
I
1sg-with
stay
‘Stay with me.’
b.
Xʊdaˑ-ɛ
gʌnɛ.
God-gen
for
‘For God’s sake!’
The occurrence of person marking may also be subject to other constraints. For instance, in Kiribati (Oceanic) there is person marking only when the referent is animate, as shown in (10a-b).
(10) Kiribati (Groves et al. 1985: 65)
a.
nako-ia
mooa
to-3pl
chickens
‘to the chickens’
b.
nako
taian
nii
to
art
coconut.trees
‘to the coconut trees’
However, borderline cases such as Spanish, which has bound pronominal forms for just one preposition, con 'with', and only for the first and second person singular forms, were coded as showing value two.
Finally, there are languages which have adpositional marking with both pronominal and nominal objects. Abkhaz (Northwest Caucasian; Georgia) is a case in point, as shown in (11).
(11) Abkhaz (Hewitt 1979: 103)
a.
a-jə̀yas
a-q’nə̀
def-river
3sg-at
‘at the river’
b.
sarà
s-q’ənt˚’
I
1sg-from
‘from me’
Such languages are assigned value four. Like value three, this value includes languages which do not normally use a full pronoun in constructions such as (11b).
In my sample there were no languages of the other logically possible type, which would have person marking on adpositions for nominal but not for pronominal objects.
The phenomenon of person marking on adpositions is not distributed evenly over the world. First, the languages of North America and Australia lack adpositions much more often than the other areas. Of the North American languages in the sample, 48% are adpositionless; for Australia the percentage is as high as 73%. The overall figure for adpositionlessness is around 17% in the current sample.
If we look only at languages with adpositions, then the following may be observed. Person marking on adpositions is non-existent in Australia, and rare in Southeast Asia (3 out of 30 languages, all belonging to the Austronesian family). For Southeast Asia this does not come as a surprise, given the general lack of person marking in this area. In Australia, on the other hand, person agreement on the verb is found in the majority of the languages, which may be an indication that the two types of person marking are not necessarily closely related to each other. In Mesoamerica and the Pacific, person marking on adpositions is clearly abundant. These are the only areas where a majority of the languages have this kind of marking: over 60%.
Other striking figures are the relatively frequent occurrence of languages of type four (person marking for pronouns and nouns) in Mesoamerica. It is above all the Mayan and Uto-Aztecan languages which have this property. In Africa, although this area has a relatively high amount of type three languages, none of the languages show type four. Eurasia, New Guinea and South America are all close to the overall distribution. Of the 20 Indo-European languages in the sample, only the two Celtic languages Irish and Welsh and the two Iranian languages Persian and Kurdish have any marking at all; they all show type three.
Due to the fact that linguists differ in the criteria that they use in determining the existence of adpositions in a language, there may be considerable discrepancies in the percentage of languages which are seen to lack adpositions altogether. My figure of 17% is close to that of Hawkins (1983), and also Tsunoda et al. (1995). By contrast, Matthew Dryer (Chapter 85 of this atlas) is more liberal in his interpretation of what constitutes an adposition and treats in some cases as adpositions what I have considered to be case clitics.
A second point that deserves a word of comment is that it is not always possible on the basis of the information presented in grammars to decide what the precise grammatical status of a specific person marker is, i.e. whether it is indeed an affix attached to the adposition, or a clitic, or even a more or less independent pronominal form. Apart from morphosyntactic information, the phonological form of the person marker may be an indication, as well as the amount of (dis)similarity to other person markers, notably independent and possessive pronouns.
A final issue which needs to be briefly mentioned is the diachronic status of the markers. They may be relatively recent, and introduced after adpositions were formed in the language. In these cases they are often phonologically related to the actual personal pronouns of the language, typically in their object or oblique form. This is the case in about 12% of the languages that display some form of person marking on their adpositions. Or the markers may be relatively old, and already present on the verbal or nominal precursor of the adposition. If there is similarity to any other person marker in the language at all, similarity to subject or object agreement markers on the verb may (but need not) be an indication of the verbal origin of the adposition. This seems to be the case in about 13% of the relevant languages. Similarity to possessive markers, which was attested in about 24% of the relevant languages, may point to a nominal origin. In around 41% of the cases, person markers on adpositions bear a similarity to both verb agreement and possessive markers in the language concerned. In the remaining 11% no clear similarities could be detected.